Technical Papers
Mar 19, 2021

Differences between Professionals and Students in Their Visual Attention on Multiple Representation Types While Solving an Open-Ended Engineering Design Problem

This article has a reply.
VIEW THE REPLY
This article has a reply.
VIEW THE REPLY
Publication: Journal of Civil Engineering Education
Volume 147, Issue 3

Abstract

Students and professionals from a variety of domains have demonstrated different approaches to problem solving. These two populations have displayed differences when using and perceiving multiple representations of problem-solving tools. In the domain of transportation engineering, this difference has yet to be evaluated in detail. This study addresses that knowledge gap. We used a mixed-methods approach with measurements of eye movements for visual attention and reflective interviews to gather participants’ reported representation use and problem-solving assumptions with three taxonomies of representations (equations, graphs, and flowchart) because they solved an open-ended design problem. Visual attention (VA) was recorded with a head-mounted eye-tracking device. Reflective interviews were used as a self-reported depiction of overt VA on the selected representation, and to record assumptions made by each participant to solve the problem. Equations, graphs, and flowcharts received different magnitudes of statistically significant VA for both groups. Professionals had a significant difference in VA between the flowchart/equation and graph representations, whereas students showed a difference in all three categories. Professionals generally chose representations with higher complexity (use of different combinations of representations) than students, as reflected by their frequency of conjoining representation choices and their associated assumptions. Professionals commonly approached problem solving by documenting specific assumptions, whereas students approached the problem more generically. Efficient information extraction occurred for professionals, but it took more time for them to solve the problem than novices. Novices frequently utilized the flowchart. Differences in performance may be explained by professionals using conceptual frameworks, or rules for interpreting and navigating the problems that were potentially developed through exposure to the domain of knowledge and deep understanding of the subject matter.

Get full access to this article

View all available purchase options and get full access to this article.

Data Availability Statement

Some or all data, models, or codes that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 1463769.

References

Abadi, M. G., S. Gestson, S. Brown, and D. S. Hurwitz. 2019. “Traffic signal phasing problem-solving rationales of professional engineers developed from eye-tracking and clinical interviews traffic signal phasing problem-solving rationales of professional engineers developed from eye-tracking and clinical interviews.” Transport Res. Rec. 2673 (4): 685–696. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361198119837506.
Ainsworth, S. 1999. “The functions of multiple representations.” Comput. Educ. 33 (2–3): 131–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0360-1315(99)00029-9.
Ali, M. 2015. “An overview of research on experts and novice problem solvers in physics.” Buletin Persatuan Pendidikan Sains dan Matematik Johor 25 (1): 70–75.
Blascheck, T., K. Kurzhals, M. Raschke, M. Burch, D. Weiskopf, and T. Ertl. 2014. “State-of-the-art of visualization for eye tracking data.” In Proc., Eurographics Conf. on Visualization (EuroVis), 1–20. Godlar, Germany: Eurographics Association.
Boshuizen, H. P. A., and H. G. Schmidt. 2008. “The development of clinical reasoning expertise.” In Clinical reasoning in the health professions, 113–121. New York: Elsevier.
Breslow, L. A., J. G. Trafton, and R. M. Ratwani. 2009. “A perceptual process approach to selecting color scales for complex visualizations.” J. Exp. Psychol. 15 (1): 25–34.
Brown, S., B. Lutz, N. Perova-Mello, and O. Ha. 2019. “Exploring differences in statics concept inventory scores among students and practitioners.” J. Eng. Educ. 108 (1): 119–135. https://doi.org/10.1002/jee.20246.
Bruder, C., and C. Hasse. 2019. “Differences between experts and novices in the monitoring of automated systems.” Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 72 (Apr): 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ergon.2019.03.007.
Chandler, P., and J. Sweller. 1992. “The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction.” Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 62 (2): 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1992.tb01017.x.
Cheville, A. 2014. “Defining engineering education.” In Proc., 121st ASEE Annual Conf. and Exposition. Washington, DC: American Society for Engineering Education.
Convertino, G., J. Chen, B. Yost, S. Ryu, and C. North. 2003. “Exploring context switching and cognition in dual-view coordinated visualizations.” In Proc., Int. Conf. on Coordinated and Multiple Views in Exploratory Visualization, 55–62. New York: IEEE.
Corbetta, M., and G. L. Shulman. 2002. “Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.” Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 (3): 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755.
Cristino, L. 2011. Bahia inicia uso de inseto transgênico contra dengue. Abingdon, UK: Taylor & Francis.
Dupont, L. 2014. “Eye-tracking analysis in landscape perception research: Influence of photograph properties and landscape characteristics.” Landsc. Res. 39 (4): 417–432. https://doi.org/10.1080/01426397.2013.773966.
Earnest, J. 2006. “ABET engineering technology criteria and competency based engineering education.” In Proc., Frontiers in Education 35th Annual Conf., 7–12. New York: IEEE.
Eye Tracking Incorporation. 2013. We’re going to need a bigger sample. River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Fasano, G., and A. Franceschini. 1987. “A multidimensional version of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test.” Mon. Not. Roy. Astron. Soc. 225 (1): 155–170. https://doi.org/10.1093/mnras/225.1.155.
Fitts, P. M., R. E. Jones, and J. L. Milton. 1950. “Eye movements of aircraft pilots during instrument-landing approaches.” Aeronaut. Eng. Rev. 9 (2): 1–6.
Frintrop, S., E. Rome, and H. I. Christensen. 2010. “Computational visual attention systems and their cognitive foundations.” ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 7 (1): 1–39. https://doi.org/10.1145/1658349.1658355.
Galesic, M., R. Tourangeau, M. P. Couper, and F. G. Conrad. 2008. “Eye-tracking data new insights on response order effects and other cognitive shortcuts in survey responding.” Publ. Opin. Q. 72 (5): 892–913. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfn059.
Gegenfurtner, A., E. Lehtinen, and R. Säljö. 2011. “Expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations : A meta-analysis of eye-tracking research in professional domains.” Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23 (4): 523–552. https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10648-011-9174-7.
Guan, Z., S. Lee, E. Cuddihy, and J. Ramey. 2006. “The validity of the stimulated retrospective think-aloud method as measured by eye tracking.” In Proc., SIGCHI Conf. on Human Factors in Computing Sustems, 1253–1262. New York: Association for Computing Machinery.
Hagerty, M., N. H. Narayanan, and P. Freitas. 2002. “Understanding machines from multimedia and hypermedia presentations.” In The psychology of science text comprehenstion, 357–384. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hornof, A., and T. Halverson. 2002. “Cleaning up systematic error in eye-tracking data by using fixation locations.” Behav. Res. Meth. Instrum. Comput. 34 (4): 592–604. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195487.
ITE (Institute of Traffic Engineers). 2000. Traffic engineering handbook. 5th ed. Washington, DC: ITE.
Jonassen, D., J. Strobel, and C. B. Lee. 2006. “Everyday problem solving in engineering: Lessons for engineering educators.” J. Eng. Educ. 95 (2): 139–151. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2168-9830.2006.tb00885.x.
Kasarskis, P., J. Stehwien, J. Hickox, A. Aretz, and C. Wickens. 2001. “Comparison of expert and novice scan behaviors during VFR flight.” In Proc., 11th Int. Symp. on Aviation Psychology. Columbus, OH: Ohio State Univ.
Kokotovich, V. 2008. “Problem analysis and thinking tools: An empirical study of non-hierarchical mind mapping.” Des. Stud. 29 (1): 49–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2007.09.001.
Korte, R., S. Brunhaver, and S. Sheppard. 2015. “(Mis)Interpretations of organizational socialization: The expectations and experiences of newcomers and managers.” Hum. Res. Dev. Q. 26 (2): 185. https://doi.org/10.1002/hrdq.21206.
Lozner, T., U. Bailey, E. Lindstrom, K. Lee, S. Quayle, S. Beaird, S. Tsoi, P. Ryus, D. Gettman, S. Sunkari, K. Balke, D. Bullock, and A. Tanaka. 2015. Signal timing manual. 2nd ed. Washington, DC: NCHRP.
Morphew, J. W., J. P. Mestre, B. H. Ross, and N. E. Strand. 2015. “Do experts and novices direct attention differently in examining physics diagrams? A study of change detection using the flicker technique.” Phys. Rev. Spec. Top. Phys. Educ. Res. 11 (2): 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevSTPER.11.020104.
Pavlović, N., and K. Jensen. 2009. “Eye tracking translation directionality.” Translat. Res. Project. 2: 93–109.
Rayner, K. 1977. “Visual attention in reading: Eye movements reflect cognitive processes.” Mem. Cognit. 5 (4): 443–448. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197383.
Rayner, K. 1998. “Eye movements in reading and information processing.” Psychol. Bull. 124 (3): 372–422. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372.
Rayner, K., and M. Castelhano. 2007. “Eye movements.” Scholarperdia 2 (10): 3649. https://doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.3649.
Roess, R. P., E. S. Prassas, and W. R. McShane. 2011. Traffic engineering. 4th ed. Upper Saddle: Prentice Hall.
Salvucci, D. D., and J. H. Goldberg. 2000. “Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols.” In Proc., Symp. on Eye Tracking Research and Applications, 71–78. New York: Association for Computing Machinery.
Schwonke, R., K. Berthold, and A. Renkl. 2009. “How multiple external representations are used and how they can be made more useful.” Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 23 (9): 1227–1243. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1526.
Shulman, G. L., R. W. Remington, and J. P. McLean. 1979. “Moving attention through visual space.” J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 5 (3): 522–526. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.5.3.522.
Snowden, R., P. Thompson, and T. Troscianko. 2012. Basic vision an introduction to visual perception. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Sofaer, S. 2002. “Qualitative research methods.” Int. J. Qual. Health Care 14 (4): 329. https://doi.org/10.1093/intqhc/14.4.329.
Sudman, S., and N. Bradburn. 2003. “Thinking about answers: The application of cognitive processes to survey methodology.” Qual. Life Res. 12 (6): 719. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025127424627.
Tien, T., P. H. Pucher, M. H. Sodergren, K. Sriskandarajah, G. Yang, and A. Darzi. 2014. “Science direct eye tracking for skills assessment and training : A systematic review.” 191 (1): 169–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jss.2014.04.032.
Tsui, A. 2003. Understanding expertise in teaching: Case studies of second language teachers. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Turochy, R. E., J. Fricker, H. G. Hawkins, D. S. Hurwitz, S. S. Ivey, M. A. Knodler, and R. K. Young. 2013. “Assessment of introductory transportation engineering course and general transportation engineering curriculum.” Transport. Res. Rec.: J. Transport. Res. Board 2328 (1): 9–15. https://doi.org/10.3141/2328-02.
Van Gog, T., F. Paas, J. J. G. Van Merrie, and P. Witte. 2005. “Uncovering the problem-solving process: Cued retrospective reporting versus concurrent and retrospective reporting.” J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl. 11 (4): 237–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.11.4.237.
Van Labeke, N., and S. E. Ainsworth. 2001. “Applying the DeFT framework to the design of multi-representational instructional simulations.” In Proc., 10th Int. Conf. on AI in Education, 314–321. San Antonio, TX: IOS Press.
Van Meeuwen, L. W., H. Jarodzka, S. Brand-Gruwel, P. A. Kirschner, J. J. P. R. De Bock, and J. J. G. Van Merriënboer. 2014. “Identification of effective visual problem solving strategies in a complex visual domain.” Learn. Instruct. 32 (Aug): 10–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.01.004.
Van Someren, M. W., P. Reimann, and H. Boshuizen. 1998. Learning with multiple representations. Advances in learning and instruction series. New York: Elsevier.
Vine, S. J., R. S. W. Masters, and J. S. Mcgrath. 2012. “Cheating experience: Guiding novices to adopt the gaze strategies of experts expedites the learning of technical laparoscopic skills.” Surgery 152 (1): 32–40.
Voßkühler, A., V. Nordmeier, L. Kuchinke, and A. M. Jacobs. 2008. “OGAMA (Open Gaze and Mouse Analyzer): Open-Source software designed to analyze eye and mouse movements in slideshow study designs.” Behav. Res. Meth. 40 (4): 1150–1162. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.4.1150.
Wolff, C. E., H. Jarodzka, and N. Van Den. Bogert. 2016. “Teacher vision: Expert and novice teachers’ perception of problematic classroom management scenes.” Instr. Sci. 44 (3): 243–265. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-016-9367-z.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Journal of Civil Engineering Education
Journal of Civil Engineering Education
Volume 147Issue 3July 2021

History

Received: Apr 7, 2020
Accepted: Dec 15, 2020
Published online: Mar 19, 2021
Published in print: Jul 1, 2021
Discussion open until: Aug 19, 2021

Permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Ananna Ahmed [email protected]
Graduate Research Assistant, School of Civil and Construction Engineering, Oregon State Univ., 101 Kearney Hall, Corvallis, OR 97333. Email: [email protected]
Associate Professor, School of Civil and Construction Engineering, Oregon State Univ., 101 Kearney Hall, Corvallis, OR 97333 (corresponding author). ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8450-6516. Email: [email protected]
Graduate Research Assistant, School of Civil and Construction Engineering, Oregon State Univ., 101 Kearney Hall, Corvallis, OR 97333. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4796-1410. Email: [email protected]
Professor, School of Civil and Construction Engineering, Oregon State Univ., 101 Kearney Hall, Corvallis, OR 97333. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3669-8407. Email: [email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Download citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

Cited by

  • Influence of Cues on the Safety Hazard Recognition of Construction Workers during Safety Training: Evidence from an Eye-Tracking Experiment, Journal of Civil Engineering Education, 10.1061/JCEECD.EIENG-1882, 150, 1, (2024).
  • Exploring the Differences in Hydraulic Engineering Problem-Solving Behavior between Undergraduate Students and Engineering Practitioners with Multiple Representations Using Eye-Tracking Techniques, Journal of Civil Engineering Education, 10.1061/JCEECD.EIENG-1872, 150, 1, (2024).
  • Teaching occupational health and safety in engineering using active learning: A systematic review, Safety Science, 10.1016/j.ssci.2023.106391, 171, (106391), (2024).
  • A Literature Review Comparing Experts’ and Non-Experts’ Visual Processing of Graphs during Problem-Solving and Learning, Education Sciences, 10.3390/educsci13020216, 13, 2, (216), (2023).
  • The effects of Multiple Representation Method and Prior Knowledge Level on Problem Solving Skills and Cognitive Load, Participatory Educational Research, 10.17275/per.23.78.10.5, 10, 5, (119-146), (2023).
  • Studying the Utilization of a Map-Based Visualization with Vitality Datasets by Domain Experts, Geographies, 10.3390/geographies2030024, 2, 3, (379-396), (2022).

View Options

Get Access

Access content

Please select your options to get access

Log in/Register Log in via your institution (Shibboleth)
ASCE Members: Please log in to see member pricing

Purchase

Save for later Information on ASCE Library Cards
ASCE Library Cards let you download journal articles, proceedings papers, and available book chapters across the entire ASCE Library platform. ASCE Library Cards remain active for 24 months or until all downloads are used. Note: This content will be debited as one download at time of checkout.

Terms of Use: ASCE Library Cards are for individual, personal use only. Reselling, republishing, or forwarding the materials to libraries or reading rooms is prohibited.
ASCE Library Card (5 downloads)
$105.00
Add to cart
ASCE Library Card (20 downloads)
$280.00
Add to cart
Buy Single Article
$35.00
Add to cart

Get Access

Access content

Please select your options to get access

Log in/Register Log in via your institution (Shibboleth)
ASCE Members: Please log in to see member pricing

Purchase

Save for later Information on ASCE Library Cards
ASCE Library Cards let you download journal articles, proceedings papers, and available book chapters across the entire ASCE Library platform. ASCE Library Cards remain active for 24 months or until all downloads are used. Note: This content will be debited as one download at time of checkout.

Terms of Use: ASCE Library Cards are for individual, personal use only. Reselling, republishing, or forwarding the materials to libraries or reading rooms is prohibited.
ASCE Library Card (5 downloads)
$105.00
Add to cart
ASCE Library Card (20 downloads)
$280.00
Add to cart
Buy Single Article
$35.00
Add to cart

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share with email

Email a colleague

Share