TECHNICAL PAPERS
Sep 1, 1994

Effect of Restriction of Vision on Driving Performance

Publication: Journal of Transportation Engineering
Volume 120, Issue 5

Abstract

The effect of restricting vision on driving performance was investigated in a field study. Commonly occurring binocular visual‐field defects were simulated for a group of young normal subjects and the effect of these defects on their driving performance on a private closed rural road, free of other vehicles, was assessed. The monocular condition did not significantly affect performance for any of the driving tasks assessed. Restriction of the binocular visual field to 40° and less, significantly increased the time taken to complete the course, reduced the ability to detect and correctly identify road signs, avoid obstacles, and to maneuver through limited spaces. Accuracy of road positioning and reversing were also impaired with field restriction. However, the time taken for many driving tasks, reversing and maneuvering, and the driver's ability to estimate speed, stopping distance, or reaction time were not affected by a restriction of the binocular visual fields. The results are discussed with regard to their impact on traffic engineering practices.

Get full access to this article

View all available purchase options and get full access to this article.

References

1.
Brandt, T., Dichans, J., and Keonig, E. (1973). “Differential effects of central versus peripheral vision on egocentric and exocentric motion perception.” Experimental Brain Res., Vol. 16, 476–491.
2.
Burg, A. (1967). “The relationship between vision test scores and driving record: general findings.” Rep. No. 67‐24, University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, Calif.
3.
Burg, A. (1968). “The relationship between vision test scores and driving record: additional findings.” Rep. No. 68‐27, UCLA, Los Angeles, Calif.
4.
Buyck, A., Missotten, L., Maes, M. J., and Van de Voorde, H. (1988). “Assessment of the driving behavior of visually handicapped persons.” Vision in vehicles II, A. G. Gale, M. H. Freeman, C. M. Haslegrave, P. Smith, and S. P. Taylor, eds., Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, Holland, 131–142.
5.
Cavallo, V., Laya, O., and Laurent, M. (1986). “The estimation of time‐to‐collision as a function of visual stimulation.” Vision in vehicles. A. G. Gale, M. H. Freeman, C. M. Haslegrave, P. Smith, and S. P. Taylor, eds., Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, Holland, 179–183.
6.
Cole, D. G. (1979). Follow‐up investigation of the visual fields and accident experience among North Carolina drivers. University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C.
7.
Council, F. M., and Allen, J. A. (1974). A study of the visual fields of North Carolina drivers and their relationship to accidents. University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C.
8.
Davison, P. A. (1978). “The role of drivers' vision in road safety.” Lighting Res. and Technol., 10(3), 125–139.
9.
Davison, P. A. (1985). “Inter‐relationships between British drivers' visual abilities, age and road accident histories.” Ophthalmic Physiological Optics, Vol. 5, 195–204.
10.
Fonda, G. (1989). “Legal blindness can be compatible with safe driving.” Ophthalmology, Vol. 96, 1457–1459.
11.
Gibson, J. J. (1968). “What gives rise to the perception of motion?” Psychology Rev., Vol. 75, 335–346.
12.
Gordon, D. A. (1966). “Perceptual basis of vehicular guidance.” Public Road, Vol. 34, 53–68.
13.
Groeger, J. A., and Brown, I. D. (1988). “Motion perception is not direct with indirect viewing systems.” Vision in vehicles II, A. G. Gale, M. H. Freeman, C. M. Haslegrave, P. Smith, and S. P. Taylor, eds., Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, Holland, 27–34.
14.
Henderson, R. L., and Burg, A. (1974). “Vision and audition in driving.” Rep. No DOT‐IIS‐801‐265, U.S. Dept. of Transp., National Highway Safety Administration, Washington, D.C.
15.
Hills, B. L. (1980). “Vision, visibility, and perception in driving.” Perception, Vol. 9, 183–216.
16.
Hills, B. L., and Burg, A. (1977). A reanalysis of Californian driver vision data: general findings, Lab. Rep. LR768, Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, Berkshire.
17.
Hofsetter, H. W. (1976). “Visual acuity and highway accidents.” J. Am. Optometric Assoc., Vol. 47, 887–893.
18.
Hughes, P. K., and Cole, B. L. (1984). “Search and attention conspicuity of road traffic control devices.” Australian Road Res., 14(1), 1–9.
19.
Johnson, C. A., and Keltner, J. L. (1983). “Incidence of visual field loss in 20,000 eyes and its relationship to driving performance.” Archives Ophthalmology, Vol. 101, 371–375.
20.
Kahneman, D., Ben‐Ishai, R., and Lotan, M. (1973). “Relation of a test of divided attention to road accidents.” J. Appl. Psychology, Vol. 58, 113–115.
21.
Keeney, A. H. (1968). “Relationship of ocular pathology and driving impairment.” Trans. Am. Acad. Ophthalmology Otolaryngology, Vol. 72, 737–740.
22.
Keeney, A. H., and Garvey, J. (1981). “The dilemma of the monocular driver.” Am. J. Ophthalmology, Vol. 91, 801–803.
23.
Kite, C. R., and King, J. N. (1961). “A survey of the factors limiting the visual fields of motor vehicle drivers in relation to minimum visual fields and visibility standards.” British J. Physiological Optics, Vol. 18, 85–107.
24.
Lewis, C. F., Blakeley, W. R., Swaroop, R., Masters, R. L., and McMurty, T. C. (1973). “Landing performance by low‐time private pilots after the sudden loss on binocular vision—Cyclops II.” Aerosp. Medicine, 44, 1241–1245.
25.
Liesmaa, M. (1973). “The influence of a drivers vision in relation to his driving.” Optician, Vol. 166, 10–13.
26.
Lovsund, P., and Hedin, A. (1986). “Effects on driving performance of visual field defects.” Vision in vehicles, A. G. Gale, M. H. Freeman, C. M. Haslegrave, P. Smith, and S. P. Taylor, eds., Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, Holland, 323–329.
27.
Mendenhall, W. (1987). Introduction to probability and statistics. PWS‐Kent & Nelson, Melbourne, Australia.
28.
Osaka, N. (1988). “Speed estimation through restricted visual field during driving in day and night: naso‐temporal hemifield differences.” Vision in vehicles II, A. G. Gale, M. H. Freeman, C. M. Haslegrave, and S. P. Taylor, eds., Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, Holland, 45–55.
29.
Retchin, S. M., Cox, J., Fox, M., and Irwin, L. (1988). “Performance‐based measurements among elderly drivers and nondrivers.” J. Am. Geriatrics Soc., Vol. 36, 813–819.
30.
Robinson, G. H., and Desai, S. D. (1971). “Control of attention as a function of risk and risk preference.” Psychological aspects of driver behavior, SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research, Versburg, The Netherlands.
31.
Shinar, D. (1977). Driver visual limitations: diagnosis and treatment. Contract DOT‐HS‐5‐1275, U.S. Department of Transportation, Washington, D.C.
32.
Smith, D. I. (1976). “Official driver records and self reports as sources of accident conviction data for research purposes.” Accident Anal. and Prevention, Vol. 8, 207–211.
33.
Wood, J. M., and Troutbeck, R. J. (1992). “The effect of restriction of the binocular visual field on driving performance.” Opthalmic & Pysiologocal Optics, Vol. 12, 291–298.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Journal of Transportation Engineering
Journal of Transportation Engineering
Volume 120Issue 5September 1994
Pages: 737 - 752

History

Received: Apr 16, 1992
Published online: Sep 1, 1994
Published in print: Sep 1994

Permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Rod Troutbeck
Assoc. Prof., Transp. Engrg., Physical Infrastructure Centre, Dept. Civ. Engrg. Queensland Univ. of Technol., Brisbane, Australia 4001
Joanne M. Wood
Sr. Lect., Centre for Eye Res., Queensland Univ. of Technology, Brisbane, Australia 4001

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Download citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

Cited by

View Options

Get Access

Access content

Please select your options to get access

Log in/Register Log in via your institution (Shibboleth)
ASCE Members: Please log in to see member pricing

Purchase

Save for later Information on ASCE Library Cards
ASCE Library Cards let you download journal articles, proceedings papers, and available book chapters across the entire ASCE Library platform. ASCE Library Cards remain active for 24 months or until all downloads are used. Note: This content will be debited as one download at time of checkout.

Terms of Use: ASCE Library Cards are for individual, personal use only. Reselling, republishing, or forwarding the materials to libraries or reading rooms is prohibited.
ASCE Library Card (5 downloads)
$105.00
Add to cart
ASCE Library Card (20 downloads)
$280.00
Add to cart
Buy Single Article
$35.00
Add to cart

Get Access

Access content

Please select your options to get access

Log in/Register Log in via your institution (Shibboleth)
ASCE Members: Please log in to see member pricing

Purchase

Save for later Information on ASCE Library Cards
ASCE Library Cards let you download journal articles, proceedings papers, and available book chapters across the entire ASCE Library platform. ASCE Library Cards remain active for 24 months or until all downloads are used. Note: This content will be debited as one download at time of checkout.

Terms of Use: ASCE Library Cards are for individual, personal use only. Reselling, republishing, or forwarding the materials to libraries or reading rooms is prohibited.
ASCE Library Card (5 downloads)
$105.00
Add to cart
ASCE Library Card (20 downloads)
$280.00
Add to cart
Buy Single Article
$35.00
Add to cart

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share with email

Email a colleague

Share